(Cross-posted from my blog, Understanding Social Change)

For those of us interested in progressive social change, things can seem demoralising sometimes. We often see setbacks, such as when the UK government announces new oil and gas licences, even when experts agree that further oil and gas exploration is not compatible with plans to keep global temperatures under 1.5 degrees, in line with the Paris Agreement. If not setbacks, painfully slow progress is also common, such as the gradual plateauing of animal consumption in the UK.

A common concept invoked by activists to keep morale high is the idea of social tipping points. In a nutshell, it is the idea that when a committed minority of people adopt a belief or behaviour, this can spread rapidly through society. It supports the idea that change can seem painfully slow for a long time, and then suddenly everything changes at once. This is reassuring for activists who have been campaigning diligently for years, with little avail. A neat graphical demonstration of this can be seen below when applied to US state legislation on various social issues (source here).

Most obviously, in issues such as same-sex marriage (characterised by the Obergefell vs Hodges case) or women’s right the vote (the 19th amendment), things moved slowly, or even sometimes in the wrong direction, for decades, before a sudden flurry of activity led to an increasing number of state-wide wins, culminating in a national level win. 

This is an inspiring story for activists and those campaigning for social change – it means it’s okay that we don’t see the fruits of our labour instantly, or even incremental wins taking us in the right direction. Our hard work may still be paying off, but social change is messy, and things take time. All of these things are almost certainly true. However, it can also lull us into a false sense of optimism, by allowing us to think that we’re winning, and results take time, where the reality is that we’re not making any meaningful progress.

There are several examples of putting a lot of belief in social tipping points in society, whether it’s in popular media outlets, social media forumsacademia or civil society groups(Since publishing this originally, Chris Bryant mentioned potential tipping points around veganism in this Veganuary piece in the Conversation - I remain sceptical due to the reasons below!) This paper, bringing a much-needed critical look at the scientific landscape of social tipping points, finds that 47% of all papers referencing social tipping points were published in 2020-2021, focusing on them as a climate solution (e.g this popular one by Simon Sharpe & Timothy Lenton).


 

I think this mentality of social tipping point-based optimism is neatly summed up by environmental journalist and campaigner George Monbiot, who says:

“​​My own belief is that our best hope is to precipitate a social tipping: widening the concentric circles of those committed to systemic change until a critical threshold is reached, that flips the status quo. Observational and experimental evidence suggests the threshold is roughly 25% of the population”. 

There’s a lot to unpack here, but I want to mainly focus on his last statement, that experimental evidence suggests the threshold for social tipping points is around 25% of a population being a committed minority and trying to overturn conventional norms. This statement comes from the 2018 paper, Experimental evidence for tipping points in social convention (un-paywalled version here), by Damon Centola et al. If that name rings a bill, Damon Centola is also the author of the popular book Change, which covered similar topics.

I have some issues with this paper, and particularly how it’s been communicated by the authors and mainstream society. Most egregious might be press releases and articles by the lead author and his university, with headlines like: “Tipping point for large-scale social change? Just 25 percent” or “The 25 Percent Tipping Point for Social Change

This has led to this 25% number popping up in lots of places, whether it’s George Monbiot’s columns, Scientific AmericanThe Atlantic, or Harvard Business Review. More anecdotally, I’ve recently heard often mentioned with climate activist circles as a reason for optimism given the slow progress on climate (possibly helped by George Monbiot’s popular Twitter thread).

So, I wanted to examine this 25% number, to see if it holds up to the big claims made above. Long story short, this paper provides extremely little evidence that 25% is a likely number for social tipping points, for issues that people actually care about.

First of all, there is the easy critique that this answer is so absurdly simple, that this cannot be true. How can we expect social change, across a variety of different issues, cultures and times, to often converge at a single, round number? This is far too simple and sadly, our world is far too complex. 

The more nuanced critique involves looking at the methodology, which is sadly not discussed enough in popular articles (although, Wired and The Atlantic to their credit, do give some discussion of the methodology and its limitations, but sadly, not enough).

The paper arrives at this 25% figure using two methods, an experimental set-up, as well as computational modelling. I’ll focus on the experimental test, given the computational modelling is very likely above my pay grade. 

In short, the experiment created 10 independent online groups, ranging from 20 to 30 people, drawn from a random pool of 194 study participants. Pairs of people would be drawn from within the 10 groups and matched, where they were assigned a photo of a face and encouraged to coordinate by guessing a name for that face. 

If both individuals in the pair guessed the same name for the face, they would get a reward of $0.10 for coordinating, and lose $0.10 if they didn’t coordinate (but they couldn’t lose money overall, obviously, otherwise that would be a hard sell to get people to join your research study). I’ve attached a screenshot from the paper’s supplementary materials below, so you can see what the participants experienced. 

Initially, the game was run until all players would reliably associate a face with a name, and there was some equilibrium reached (e.g. Mary above). 

How does this actually answer the question of when we should expect social tipping points to occur? The answer lies in the (clever) experimental design that comes next, where each of the 10 groups was now injected with some variable percentage of confederates, or a “committed minority”, who all wanted to give an alternative name for the same face (e.g. Sarah, rather than Mary). For example, the percentage of confederates trying to overturn the established norm varied in these 10 groups from 17% to 31% of the total population. Using this, Centola et al. could mathematically observe at what percentage of the total population the broader group shifted from their pre-established norm to their new norm (e.g., from Mary to Sarah).

Centola et al. find that this percentage where the committed minority would overturn the established norm (read: the name people were originally assigning to this random face) was around 25% of the total population, hence this neat 25% finding. You can see a nice graphic of the results below (and as before, the full paper here).

Let me start by saying I think this is a very neat experimental design, and it’s certainly interesting. The issue I have with this paper is the claims that this will generalise, in any way, to broad social issues people are referring to, whether it’s climate change, meat consumption, anti-racism, or something else.

Hopefully, now that people have seen the experimental design, it will be fairly obvious why I think this paper is far too artificial to be of much real-world relevance. However, I’ll pull out a few key points as to why I think this approach is far too simplistic to be meaningful:

  1. Assigning names to random faces is not representative of the issues we care about. Specifically, when talking about changing attitudes or behaviours, they are often tied to things that are culturally or societally ingrained e.g. eating meat is part of our tradition, flying for leisure is totally acceptable, we all aspire to own a car, and so on. These beliefs and behaviours are much harder to detach than “Oh, I called this random face Mary last time and these people really want it to change to Sarah, so I may as well go along with it”. This, most importantly, I think devalues the applicability of this research to real-world issues. Even in a much simpler dynamic of picking reusable mugs over single-use cups, research finds a tipping point of between 50-65%, far higher than the 25% proposed by Centola et al. 
  2. People were incentivised to coordinate – this doesn’t happen in reality. In this game, people were given $0.10 every time they coordinated, which, as you might expect, probably has a very positive effect on coordination. When people are incentivised by the experiment, it’s impossible to tell whether people actually changed their beliefs, or they are simply pretending to receive additional cash rewards.
    1. In reality, this is almost the polar opposite of what we might expect for some of the social issues we care about. For example, electric vehicles are often more expensive than internal combustion engine vehicles, and planet-and-animal-friendly meat alternatives are more expensive than animal products. So, people are disincentivised from adopting these pro-social behaviours, rather than incentivised to adopt them. 

3. Life isn’t a series of one-shot interactions. I’m stealing this point from this great blog post by Peter Licari, which also provides a reality check on this paper and the way it was framed. In short, this study works by pairing people up in 1-on-1 scenarios, whereas this is a relatively rare interaction in society. In reality, we build our ideas of social norms and acceptable behaviour by talking to family and friends (often in groups), reading social media (e.g. Twitter, where everyone can join in), or reading the media (where it’s a one-to-many interaction). 

 

Overall, I think this is a neat paper, and I would be very happy if I published it! However, I’m worried that people read the headlines, fail to understand the methodology and develop some ill-placed belief that social tipping points will solve the world’s issues. Of course, they might, but this paper certainly doesn’t make me feel much more confident about it. 

I’m not trying to be a doomer here. I think progress on some of these very challenging problems, be it climate change or the trillions of animals killed for food each year, is possible. However, I believe we need to have a level-headed assessment of how our issues are progressing, be it negative or positive, to better strategise and achieve change. 

--

(P.S. Damon Centola’s book Change, which covers similar topics, is a much more reasonable and non-hyperbolic overview of some of the literature and power of social networks. )

11

1
0
1
1

Reactions

1
0
1
1

More posts like this

There are no more recommendations left.

Comments1


Sorted by Click to highlight new comments since: Today at 11:08 PM

Thanks for writing and sharing! This was a useful debunking of something I hear a lot too.

Tipping points may be overrated, but I still think they can be appreciable force. Here are two reasons why:

  1. New information can lead to sudden and dramatic updates in societal opinions/actions. I'm not well read on this literature, but Leonardo Bursztyn's work on social norms speaks to these issues. In one study, he finds simply providing information on Saudi Arabian men's actual attitudes towards women working outside of the home increases their likelihood to accept services to help their wives find work by 36% (9 percentage points). In another study, he and his coauthors find that the share that the number of people willing to publicly donate to an anti-immigration group rose from 20% to 33% when given information that Trump won the county in question (as opposed to Clinton).
  2. Legislative change occurs at thresholds. As a result:
    1. Legislative thresholds naturally form "tipping points" where big changes cascade from smaller changes in public opinion.
    2. Legislative change itself abruptly provides new information about societal values, fueling further change in public opinion.

I think these kinds of dynamics underlie the rapid legislative victories outlined in the first chart in your blog post, although Loving vs. Viriginia is an interesting counterexample).

What I do find lacking from a lot of tipping point discussion is more consideration of the underlying mechanism supposedly fueling the self-propelling tendency. In new technology adoption, it is often more clear ― increasing returns to scale on the production and consumption side are a big one. For social norms, for me at least it's often less clear how, where and why beliefs propagate in society. 

Curated and popular this week
 · 2d ago · 1m read
 · 
Hello FAST! We have good news this month: new cage-free listings in Peru! 1- HOTEL RESTAURANTS EL ALBERGUE consists of two restaurants and a high-end hotel in Ollantaytambo, Cusco Links: https://www.instagram.com/p/DHrNDEWyP8e/ Scale: National - Peru Who: Arba Failed tactics: None Successful tactics: Working from the beginning of the conversations with the entire team (management, logistics, kitchen, administration) thus generating a successful communication channel from each area. Scalability: This group is a huge cultural and gastronomic landmark in the Ollantaytambo Valley in Cusco, on the way to Machu Picchu. We are approaching new hotel groups in the area. Follow-up: We will stay in touch to request reports on its implementation. 2- RESTAURANT EL CHUNCHO Part of the El Albergue investment group https://www.instagram.com/p/DHonGYGygbn/ Scale: National - Peru Who: Arba Failed tactics: None Successful tactics: Working from the beginning of conversations with the entire team (management, logistics, kitchen, administration) thus generating a successful communication channel from each area. Scalability: This group is a huge cultural and gastronomic landmark in the Ollantaytambo Valley in Cusco, on the way to Machu Picchu. We are reaching out to new hotel groups in the area. Follow-up: We will stay in touch to request reports on its implementation. 3- BICHOS SPECIALTY COFFEE https://www.instagram.com/p/DHCEK_pxXmV/ Scale: National - Peru Who: Arba Failed tactics: None Successful tactics: Excellent communication from the start Scalability: This group is a huge culinary icon in Huancayo, Junín department, Peru. This is the first commitment of the area. Follow-up: We will stay in touch to request reports on its implementation. 4- DOPAMINE https://www.instagram.com/p/DGOLiz3OV1W/?img_index=2&igsh=MXgweHZzbW5obXNiYw%3D%3D Scale: National - Peru Who: Arba Failed tactics: None Successful tactics: Excellent communication from the start Scalability: New style of cafe, a un
Dydanimal
 · 4d ago · 1m read
 · 
Hello Fast! This year, the new aquaculture law will begin to be created, and we have launched several campaigns to raise awareness about marine animals and bring them closer to the public. One of these is our Save the Lobsters (Salvemos a las Langostas) campaign. A major Chilean supermarket (with locations in several Latin American countries) displays lobsters for sale in small aquariums that violate basic animal welfare standards and, furthermore, lack an internal protocol for their sale. In response, we have taken various actions, one of which is to alert the public. We have created a website to raise awareness about the situation, recently launched a petition for signatures, and as a result of our efforts, the Colegio Médico Veterinario de Chile (Chilean Veterinary Medical Association) has issued a statement supporting the view that invertebrates are sentient beings! This is very encouraging for us, as this is a highly respected professional body in Chile. We invite you to learn more about our campaign and reach out to us for any feedback you may have. We would be happy to hear from you.   Best regards, Marysabel Pavez Director Fundación Derecho y Defensa Animal (DyDAnimal Chile) marysabel@dydanimal.com
 · 5d ago · 1m read
 · 
Hi FAST!  The fight against fur farming in Sweden has taken yet another step forward.  This past week, the Swedish government has officially launched an inquiry into how a fur farming ban can be implemented. This follows a commitment made in 2023 and is the result of over 50 years of advocacy by Project 1882.  Last year, the government pledged to phase out mink farms and explore a full ban on fur farming. To support this transition, financial aid was introduced earlier this year to help farmers shut down their operations. Now, with this inquiry in motion, Sweden is closer than ever to ending fur farming for good. A legal ban is essential to ensure fur farming never returns.  The inquiry will outline how the ban will be enforced, with “animal ethics discussions” cited as a reason for moving forward. With the last mink farms expected to close to qualify for the government’s decommissioning support of 180 million SEK, industry resistance is fading, making a ban easier to implement.  Now, Project 1882 is urging the Swedish government to move forward with the inquiry without delay and to push for an EU-wide ban in line with the Fur Free Europe citizens’ initiative.  Read more here: https://www.project1882.org/news/major-victory-sweden-moves-closer-ban-fur-farming  
Shy38inc
 · 6d ago · 1m read
 · 
Shy 38 Inc. Kansas Farm Sanctuary along with AnimaLearn and United Poultry Concerns are launching the sanctuary's new humane alternative to classroom chick hatching: The Hatching Kindness Project! The program is free for educators and is meant to replace live chick hatching in elementary schools. It includes a complete curriculum, chicken and egg models, books, an app and access to our Chicken TV Livestream where students can at any time peek in to view their "adopted flock" and observe natural chicken behavior. For more info, (and to request the program) visit our website: https://www.shy38.org/hatching-kindness We will be presenting The Hatching Kindness Project at the APHE National Humane Education Conference in Kansas City in April.
 · 3d ago · 1m read
 · 
Hi FAST Forum, Greener by Default is hosting a webinar for culinary leaders looking to navigate the current egg price volatility and supply chain disruptions through simple, plant-based switches. The webinar will feature a panel of seasoned culinary directors who will be sharing their tips, experiences, and tested, practical solutions to help attendees protect their operations from the impacts of the egg crisis. 📅 April 7, 2025 ⏰ 12 pm ET / 9 am PT 🎯 Perfect for: Foodservice directors, chefs, and menu planners 💻 Register here: https://us06web.zoom.us/meeting/register/zJnfBADkSAyAtmxsJj3HKg  You--and anyone in your network you feel would benefit!--are welcome to join us and learn how innovative operators and chefs are maintaining quality and consistency and delighting diners—all while reducing costs through smart plant-based substitutions. Our Panel: * Chef Nina Curtis (Speaker and Moderator) - Director and Executive Chef at Plant'ish & Co. Culinary Arts * Chef Cady Frazier (Speaker) - Executive Chef at Cherry Creek School District * Rob Morasco C.E.C. (Speaker) - Vice President, Innovation at Sodexo Campus In this 60-minute session, attendees will discover: * Real success stories from operators who've already made the switch * Practical tips for reformulating desserts, dressings, and other staples * Tips for maintaining taste and texture, while reducing supply chain risk * Strategies for avoiding potential challenges and pitfalls * Easy steps to bring these strategies to life for your operation BONUS: All registrants will receive a guide to successfully making plant-based egg swaps—including a helpful plant-based egg alternative conversion chart—courtesy of Chef Nina Curtis! Please feel free to share this webinar with your networks! The registration link can be found here, and if you'd like to quickly share, GBD's post on LinkedIn is here. Thank you!  
jennycanham
 · 5d ago · 1m read
 · 
Animal Outlook has launched its first Behavior Change and Volunteer Impact report, showing the results of programs that aim to influence lasting behavior change. Produced in collaboration with User-Friendly, the report documents some of the ways we can create measurable progress in shifting public attitudes and behaviors towards vegan eating. The report also shows the impact of volunteers in helping to maximize efforts in the animal protection movement. Last year, Animal Outlook volunteers completed almost 6,000 hours of work for animals. Read the full report here: https://animaloutlook.org/wp-content/uploads/2025/03/Final-Behavior-Change-and-Volunteer-Impact-Report-2024.pdf  Coverage in Vegconomist: https://vegconomist.com/market-and-trends/animal-outlook-finds-behavior-change-strategies-volunteer-efforts-fuel-growth-vegan-diets/