Research & Data
Frontpage

(Cross-posted from my blog, Understanding Social Change)

For those of us interested in progressive social change, things can seem demoralising sometimes. We often see setbacks, such as when the UK government announces new oil and gas licences, even when experts agree that further oil and gas exploration is not compatible with plans to keep global temperatures under 1.5 degrees, in line with the Paris Agreement. If not setbacks, painfully slow progress is also common, such as the gradual plateauing of animal consumption in the UK.

A common concept invoked by activists to keep morale high is the idea of social tipping points. In a nutshell, it is the idea that when a committed minority of people adopt a belief or behaviour, this can spread rapidly through society. It supports the idea that change can seem painfully slow for a long time, and then suddenly everything changes at once. This is reassuring for activists who have been campaigning diligently for years, with little avail. A neat graphical demonstration of this can be seen below when applied to US state legislation on various social issues (source here).

Most obviously, in issues such as same-sex marriage (characterised by the Obergefell vs Hodges case) or women’s right the vote (the 19th amendment), things moved slowly, or even sometimes in the wrong direction, for decades, before a sudden flurry of activity led to an increasing number of state-wide wins, culminating in a national level win. 

This is an inspiring story for activists and those campaigning for social change – it means it’s okay that we don’t see the fruits of our labour instantly, or even incremental wins taking us in the right direction. Our hard work may still be paying off, but social change is messy, and things take time. All of these things are almost certainly true. However, it can also lull us into a false sense of optimism, by allowing us to think that we’re winning, and results take time, where the reality is that we’re not making any meaningful progress.

There are several examples of putting a lot of belief in social tipping points in society, whether it’s in popular media outlets, social media forumsacademia or civil society groups(Since publishing this originally, Chris Bryant mentioned potential tipping points around veganism in this Veganuary piece in the Conversation - I remain sceptical due to the reasons below!) This paper, bringing a much-needed critical look at the scientific landscape of social tipping points, finds that 47% of all papers referencing social tipping points were published in 2020-2021, focusing on them as a climate solution (e.g this popular one by Simon Sharpe & Timothy Lenton).


 

I think this mentality of social tipping point-based optimism is neatly summed up by environmental journalist and campaigner George Monbiot, who says:

“​​My own belief is that our best hope is to precipitate a social tipping: widening the concentric circles of those committed to systemic change until a critical threshold is reached, that flips the status quo. Observational and experimental evidence suggests the threshold is roughly 25% of the population”. 

There’s a lot to unpack here, but I want to mainly focus on his last statement, that experimental evidence suggests the threshold for social tipping points is around 25% of a population being a committed minority and trying to overturn conventional norms. This statement comes from the 2018 paper, Experimental evidence for tipping points in social convention (un-paywalled version here), by Damon Centola et al. If that name rings a bill, Damon Centola is also the author of the popular book Change, which covered similar topics.

I have some issues with this paper, and particularly how it’s been communicated by the authors and mainstream society. Most egregious might be press releases and articles by the lead author and his university, with headlines like: “Tipping point for large-scale social change? Just 25 percent” or “The 25 Percent Tipping Point for Social Change

This has led to this 25% number popping up in lots of places, whether it’s George Monbiot’s columns, Scientific AmericanThe Atlantic, or Harvard Business Review. More anecdotally, I’ve recently heard often mentioned with climate activist circles as a reason for optimism given the slow progress on climate (possibly helped by George Monbiot’s popular Twitter thread).

So, I wanted to examine this 25% number, to see if it holds up to the big claims made above. Long story short, this paper provides extremely little evidence that 25% is a likely number for social tipping points, for issues that people actually care about.

First of all, there is the easy critique that this answer is so absurdly simple, that this cannot be true. How can we expect social change, across a variety of different issues, cultures and times, to often converge at a single, round number? This is far too simple and sadly, our world is far too complex. 

The more nuanced critique involves looking at the methodology, which is sadly not discussed enough in popular articles (although, Wired and The Atlantic to their credit, do give some discussion of the methodology and its limitations, but sadly, not enough).

The paper arrives at this 25% figure using two methods, an experimental set-up, as well as computational modelling. I’ll focus on the experimental test, given the computational modelling is very likely above my pay grade. 

In short, the experiment created 10 independent online groups, ranging from 20 to 30 people, drawn from a random pool of 194 study participants. Pairs of people would be drawn from within the 10 groups and matched, where they were assigned a photo of a face and encouraged to coordinate by guessing a name for that face. 

If both individuals in the pair guessed the same name for the face, they would get a reward of $0.10 for coordinating, and lose $0.10 if they didn’t coordinate (but they couldn’t lose money overall, obviously, otherwise that would be a hard sell to get people to join your research study). I’ve attached a screenshot from the paper’s supplementary materials below, so you can see what the participants experienced. 

Initially, the game was run until all players would reliably associate a face with a name, and there was some equilibrium reached (e.g. Mary above). 

How does this actually answer the question of when we should expect social tipping points to occur? The answer lies in the (clever) experimental design that comes next, where each of the 10 groups was now injected with some variable percentage of confederates, or a “committed minority”, who all wanted to give an alternative name for the same face (e.g. Sarah, rather than Mary). For example, the percentage of confederates trying to overturn the established norm varied in these 10 groups from 17% to 31% of the total population. Using this, Centola et al. could mathematically observe at what percentage of the total population the broader group shifted from their pre-established norm to their new norm (e.g., from Mary to Sarah).

Centola et al. find that this percentage where the committed minority would overturn the established norm (read: the name people were originally assigning to this random face) was around 25% of the total population, hence this neat 25% finding. You can see a nice graphic of the results below (and as before, the full paper here).

Let me start by saying I think this is a very neat experimental design, and it’s certainly interesting. The issue I have with this paper is the claims that this will generalise, in any way, to broad social issues people are referring to, whether it’s climate change, meat consumption, anti-racism, or something else.

Hopefully, now that people have seen the experimental design, it will be fairly obvious why I think this paper is far too artificial to be of much real-world relevance. However, I’ll pull out a few key points as to why I think this approach is far too simplistic to be meaningful:

  1. Assigning names to random faces is not representative of the issues we care about. Specifically, when talking about changing attitudes or behaviours, they are often tied to things that are culturally or societally ingrained e.g. eating meat is part of our tradition, flying for leisure is totally acceptable, we all aspire to own a car, and so on. These beliefs and behaviours are much harder to detach than “Oh, I called this random face Mary last time and these people really want it to change to Sarah, so I may as well go along with it”. This, most importantly, I think devalues the applicability of this research to real-world issues. Even in a much simpler dynamic of picking reusable mugs over single-use cups, research finds a tipping point of between 50-65%, far higher than the 25% proposed by Centola et al. 
  2. People were incentivised to coordinate – this doesn’t happen in reality. In this game, people were given $0.10 every time they coordinated, which, as you might expect, probably has a very positive effect on coordination. When people are incentivised by the experiment, it’s impossible to tell whether people actually changed their beliefs, or they are simply pretending to receive additional cash rewards.
    1. In reality, this is almost the polar opposite of what we might expect for some of the social issues we care about. For example, electric vehicles are often more expensive than internal combustion engine vehicles, and planet-and-animal-friendly meat alternatives are more expensive than animal products. So, people are disincentivised from adopting these pro-social behaviours, rather than incentivised to adopt them. 

3. Life isn’t a series of one-shot interactions. I’m stealing this point from this great blog post by Peter Licari, which also provides a reality check on this paper and the way it was framed. In short, this study works by pairing people up in 1-on-1 scenarios, whereas this is a relatively rare interaction in society. In reality, we build our ideas of social norms and acceptable behaviour by talking to family and friends (often in groups), reading social media (e.g. Twitter, where everyone can join in), or reading the media (where it’s a one-to-many interaction). 

 

Overall, I think this is a neat paper, and I would be very happy if I published it! However, I’m worried that people read the headlines, fail to understand the methodology and develop some ill-placed belief that social tipping points will solve the world’s issues. Of course, they might, but this paper certainly doesn’t make me feel much more confident about it. 

I’m not trying to be a doomer here. I think progress on some of these very challenging problems, be it climate change or the trillions of animals killed for food each year, is possible. However, I believe we need to have a level-headed assessment of how our issues are progressing, be it negative or positive, to better strategise and achieve change. 

--

(P.S. Damon Centola’s book Change, which covers similar topics, is a much more reasonable and non-hyperbolic overview of some of the literature and power of social networks. )

11

1
0
1
1

Reactions

1
0
1
1

More posts like this

There are no more recommendations left.

Comments1


Sorted by Click to highlight new comments since: Today at 1:55 AM

Thanks for writing and sharing! This was a useful debunking of something I hear a lot too.

Tipping points may be overrated, but I still think they can be appreciable force. Here are two reasons why:

  1. New information can lead to sudden and dramatic updates in societal opinions/actions. I'm not well read on this literature, but Leonardo Bursztyn's work on social norms speaks to these issues. In one study, he finds simply providing information on Saudi Arabian men's actual attitudes towards women working outside of the home increases their likelihood to accept services to help their wives find work by 36% (9 percentage points). In another study, he and his coauthors find that the share that the number of people willing to publicly donate to an anti-immigration group rose from 20% to 33% when given information that Trump won the county in question (as opposed to Clinton).
  2. Legislative change occurs at thresholds. As a result:
    1. Legislative thresholds naturally form "tipping points" where big changes cascade from smaller changes in public opinion.
    2. Legislative change itself abruptly provides new information about societal values, fueling further change in public opinion.

I think these kinds of dynamics underlie the rapid legislative victories outlined in the first chart in your blog post, although Loving vs. Viriginia is an interesting counterexample).

What I do find lacking from a lot of tipping point discussion is more consideration of the underlying mechanism supposedly fueling the self-propelling tendency. In new technology adoption, it is often more clear ― increasing returns to scale on the production and consumption side are a big one. For social norms, for me at least it's often less clear how, where and why beliefs propagate in society. 

Curated and popular this week
 · 3d ago · 1m read
 · 
Hello FAST members! I am bringing you the 2024 results of our project "Reducing Anemia with Plant-Based Iron." Once again, the results are excellent. We are starting work with new schools in 2025. Project description and results: Anemia is a common disease in Peru, especially among children and women of reproductive age. Iron deficiency anemia is characterized by a lack of iron in the blood, which causes a reduction in the number of red blood cells and, therefore, a decrease in the body's ability to transport oxygen from the lungs to the various organs and tissues. In Peru, anemia is primarily due to a diet deficient in iron-rich foods and a lack of access to quality health services, which leads to infections that inhibit the absorption of this nutrient. Many people, especially in very low-income rural areas, do not have access to iron-rich foods or lack knowledge on the subject. Furthermore, access to quality health services is limited, making the diagnosis and treatment of anemia difficult. According to the Peruvian Ministry of Health, 49% of children under 5 years of age living in rural areas suffer from anemia. In urban areas, the percentage is 37%. Furthermore, 57% of women of reproductive age also suffer from this disease. To address the problem of anemia, ARBA has created a program called "Reducing Anemia with Plant-Based Iron." Through in-person and virtual workshops, it teaches parents where to obtain iron from plant-based foods and helps prevent anemia in their children. Through partnerships with schools, our team, with the consent of parents and teachers, takes blood samples from children between 4 and 12 years of age in two stages (before and after parent training). The results of these analyses are revealing and show a clear and severe iron deficiency in many children. In the two years that ARBA has been carrying out this intervention in schools, incredible results have been obtained, demonstrating that anemia can be overcome with proper nutriti
 · 2d ago · 1m read
 · 
Hey Everyone,  Posting this to share some notes from CattleCon 2025. The notes cover the talks from the event on sustainability, healthy diets and key policy issues for the industry. Hopefully they should be interesting to anyone working on those areas. Some Context: CattleCon is the annual meeting of National Cattlemen’s Beef Association (NCBA), the event includes presentations from NCBA staff, educational talks, a trade show and meetings of Beef Checkoff committees.  The NCBA is a marketing organisation and trade association founded in 1898. Its mission is: “To serve the cattle and beef industry by improving the business climate, growing beef demand, and increasing the world’s access to U.S. beef.”  According to 990s the NCBA's annual budget is around $55 million with the majority of its revenue coming from the Beef Checkoff. The notes are here: https://drive.proton.me/urls/8ENZ5T6KZ4#2onjMymrt6jN  If you have any questions about the report please get in touch. 
 · 16h ago · 1m read
 · 
Hello FAST!  I am glad to inform that the cruise line company Viking Line has decided to adopt the European Chicken Commitment (ECC). This decision follows a long dialogue with Project 1882 and encompasses all of the company's operations in Sweden, Finland, and Estonia. This makes Viking Line the latest cruise line company to commit to the ECC following discussions with Project 1882. They join Finnlines, Stena Line, and Destination Gotland in this initiative. Project 1882 is now focusing on the remaining ferry companies in the Baltic Sea, such as Tallink Silja and TT-Line. Approximately 6 million people travel with Viking Line each year.  Read more here: Viking Line says no to Frankenchickens | Project 1882
Ronen Bar
 · 2d ago · 10m read
 · 
"Part one of our challenge is to solve the technical alignment problem, and that’s what everybody focuses on, but part two is: to whose values do you align the system once you’re capable of doing that, and that may turn out to be an even harder problem", Sam Altman, OpenAI CEO (Link).  In this post, I argue that: 1. "To whose values do you align the system" is a critically neglected space I termed “Moral Alignment.” Only a few organizations work for non-humans in this field, with a total budget of 4-5 million USD (not accounting for academic work). The scale of this space couldn’t be any bigger - the intersection between the most revolutionary technology ever and all sentient beings. While tractability remains uncertain, there is some promising positive evidence (See “The Tractability Open Question” section). 2. Given the first point, our movement must attract more resources, talent, and funding to address it. The goal is to value align AI with caring about all sentient beings: humans, animals, and potential future digital minds. In other words, I argue we should invest much more in promoting a sentient-centric AI. The problem What is Moral Alignment? AI alignment focuses on ensuring AI systems act according to human intentions, emphasizing controllability and corrigibility (adaptability to changing human preferences). However, traditional alignment often ignores the ethical implications for all sentient beings. Moral Alignment, as part of the broader AI alignment and AI safety spaces, is a field focused on the values we aim to instill in AI. I argue that our goal should be to ensure AI is a positive force for all sentient beings. Currently, as far as I know, no overarching organization, terms, or community unifies Moral Alignment (MA) as a field with a clear umbrella identity. While specific groups focus individually on animals, humans, or digital minds, such as AI for Animals, which does excellent community-building work around AI and animal welfare while
Alex Romijn
 · 1d ago · 1m read
 · 
More information:  https://www.dutchnews.nl/2025/04/dolfinarium-is-making-clownsof-animals-in-shows-agency-claims/ More (in dutch): https://www.dolfinariumvrij.nl/maar-liefst-20-overtredingen-van-de-vergunning/  At 20 points the Dolphinarium in Harderwijk is in breach of the law. They have to stop or change their shows under pressure from paying fines.  We, dutch animal rights organisation Bite BAck, got a lot of media attention today.  All the big media and newspapers had an article about. We got a lot of interviews too.  A lot of companies already stopped with offering discount tickets for the Dolphinarium. We think this news will add more companies to the list.  www.biteback.nl https://www.biteback.nl